Elsevier

The Leadership Quarterly

Volume 25, Issue 5, October 2014, Pages 817-834
The Leadership Quarterly

The many (distinctive) faces of leadership: Inferring leadership domain from facial appearance

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.06.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Previous research has shown that people form impressions of potential leaders from their faces and that certain facial features predict success in reaching prestigious leadership positions. However, much less is known about the accuracy or meta-accuracy of face-based leadership inferences. Here we examine a simple, but important, question: Can leadership domain be inferred from faces? We find that human judges can identify business, military, and sports leaders (but not political leaders) from their faces with above-chance accuracy. However, people are surprisingly bad at evaluating their own performance on this judgment task: We find no relationship between how well judges think they performed and their actual accuracy levels. In a follow-up study, we identify several basic dimensions of evaluation that correlate with face-based judgments of leadership domain, as well as those that predict actual leadership domain. We discuss the implications of our results for leadership perception and selection.

Introduction

Understanding the factors that predict leader selection is clearly important: A leader influences the achievements of his/her organization and, by extension, the well-being of its members and all those who benefit (or suffer) from the organization's output. Therefore, organizations and their members should have strong incentives to identify and select effective leaders within their domain, namely by relying on objective indicators of leadership quality. Yet, the human mind often relies on superficial cues to form judgments or make decisions, and the choice of which leader to select is no exception: A large and growing literature shows that facial appearances predict success in reaching prestigious leadership positions (Antonakis and Jacquart, 2013, Olivola and Todorov, 2010a). In the domain of politics, numerous studies have found that more competent-looking political candidates garner larger vote shares (e.g., Antonakis and Dalgas, 2009, Ballew and Todorov, 2007, Poutvaara et al., 2009; for a review of this literature, see Olivola & Todorov, 2010a). Voters also seem to favor more attractive candidates (Berggren et al., 2010, Efran and Patterson, 1974) and those who look stereotypically like members of their preferred political party (Olivola, Sussman, Tsetsos, Kang, & Todorov, 2012). Similarly, in the domain of business, studies have found that CEOs who possess certain facial features command higher salaries and are hired by more successful companies (Graham et al., 2014, Harms et al., 2012, Livingston and Pearce, 2009, Pfann et al., 2000, Rule and Ambady, 2008, Wong et al., 2011). And in the military domain, facial dominance was found to predict military rank (Mazur et al., 1984, Mueller and Mazur, 1996, Mueller and Mazur, 1997; although see Loehr & O'Hara, 2013, for evidence that facial morphological correlates of dominance and aggression negatively predict military rank). In sum, there is ample research demonstrating associations, within several leadership domains (politics, business, military, etc.), between certain facial characteristics and success.3 Leaders in a particular domain (e.g., politics) who possess the “right” facial features (e.g., a competent-looking face) tend to be more successful within that domain (e.g., receive more votes) than other (potential) leaders in the same domain who do not possess those features, ceteris paribus.

While the relationship between facial appearance and success within leadership domains is now well established, much less is known about the relationship between facial appearance and selection into particular leadership domains. That is, are certain (visible) facial features associated with being a leader in one domain rather than another? Or, to put it differently, can people discriminate between leaders in one domain (e.g., military leaders) and those in another (e.g., business leaders), just by looking at their faces? This question is important: If leaders in a particular domain share facial features that distinguish them from leaders in other domains, this suggests that domain-specific facial stereotypes may also influence the leadership selection process, above-and-beyond facial cues that are broadly associated with leadership success across several domains (e.g., attractiveness and facial competence). Identifying such domain-specific facial stereotypes would therefore add a new “layer” to the role of face-based inferences in leadership selection.

This paper contributes to this important question in four ways. First, we determine whether people can accurately judge leadership domain from facial cues. To do so, we presented judges with the faces of leaders drawn from four different domains (business, military, politics, and sports) and asked them to infer which domain these leaders belong to. While there is an extensive literature on the (in)accuracy of appearance-based first impressions (e.g., Hassin and Trope, 2000, Olivola and Todorov, 2010b, Zebrowitz and Collins, 1997, Zebrowitz and Montepare, 2008), only a small fraction of these studies have specifically looked at judgments about leaders. Moreover, these studies have either examined the ability of judges to infer specific characteristics about leaders within a particular domain, such as their political orientation (e.g., Carpinella and Johnson, 2013, Jahoda, 1954, Olivola et al., 2012, Olivola and Todorov, 2010b, Wänke et al., 2012), or their ability to determine whether or not someone is a leader (Cherulnik, Turns, & Wilderman, 1990).4 We know of no studies that have asked participants to infer which domain a leader belongs to, solely from facial cues.

Second, we examine whether some leadership categories (military leaders, business leaders, etc.) are more easily identified (from facial cues) than others. In particular, we compared the accuracy of face-based leadership inferences across different leadership domains. Studies comparing face-based inferences across domains (e.g., Hassin and Trope, 2000, Olivola and Todorov, 2010b) have found that these judgments vary considerably in their accuracy levels. We might therefore expect that some leadership category inferences will be more accurate than others. In particular, it would be interesting to see whether leaders who are elected by the general population (e.g., U.S. state Governors) have more or less distinct faces than those who are selected by a smaller group of expert members within their domain (e.g., U.S. Army Generals). On the one hand, we might predict that experts, being more knowledgeable (about their respective domains), would be less influenced by superficial appearance cues than most voters (Lenz & Lawson, 2011). On the other hand, since elite members of the same organization tend to be more like-minded than the general voting population, they may be more likely to share common (but possibly erroneous) stereotypes about what good leaders in their domain look like, and therefore to select leaders who possess certain, distinctive facial features. We return to this question, below, after we present the four leadership categories in our study.

Third, we assess the meta-accuracy of face-based leadership judgments – how well people can evaluate their own ability to draw (correct) inferences from facial stimuli. Specifically, we asked our participant–judges to report their confidence in each judgment and to estimate their overall accuracy. We then compared these estimates with their actual likelihoods of correctly inferring leadership category. Research on the validity of face-based inferences has focused, almost exclusively, on the narrow question of accuracy (see Olivola & Todorov, 2010b for a critical discussion of this issue). In contrast, much less attention has been paid to the correspondence (if any) between the confidence that people hold in their face-based judgments (subjective accuracy) and their actual likelihood of being correct (objective accuracy). Yet meta-accuracy is an essential component of judgment validity since it determines whether (and when) one relies on appearances to form impressions: Regardless of their actual (i.e., objective) accuracy-levels, individuals who doubt their ability to draw useful inferences from faces are unlikely to deliberately rely on these judgments (and they risk ignoring a potentially useful social cue), whereas those who trust their first impressions are more likely to do so (and they risk giving these inferences too much weight).5 Consequently, the relative weight that individuals place on their first impressions of leaders can impact organizational dynamics, including a leader's ability to exert influence (we return to this point in Section 10). Therefore, an important goal for researchers should be to understand, not just whether human judges can (on average) draw accurate inferences from facial cues, but also the extent to which people recognize whether (as a general rule) and when (depending on the situation) they should rely on these inferences or refrain from doing so. Those few studies that did compare the accuracy and confidence associated with first impressions tended to find that judges were poorly calibrated in their self-evaluations (Ames et al., 2010, Hassin and Trope, 2000). We might therefore predict low levels of meta-accuracy in leadership category inferences. On the other hand, given the sizeable stakes involved in selecting or interacting with leaders – in particular, the high costs of relying on invalid cues and/or failing to rely on valid cues when evaluating a (potential) leader – we might expect judges to be cognizant of their ability (or lack thereof) to infer leadership characteristics from appearances.

Finally, we attempt to identify the basic dimensions of evaluation (e.g., how competent a leader looks) that correlate with face-based inferences of leadership domain. That is, we examine which dimensions of evaluation might potentially underlie inferences concerning leadership domain, as well as those that may actually “give away” a leader's domain. In particular, we had the leaders' faces rated on a variety of personality traits and physical characteristics, and we correlated these ratings with guesses about leadership domain (subjective categorizations), as well as the actual domains of the leaders in our sample (objective categories). To the extent that a particular dimension of evaluation (e.g., facial competence) simultaneously distinguishes (i) leaders who look (stereotypically) like they belong to their domain from those (in the same domain) who do not and (ii) leaders in one domain from those in another, this judgment variable might underlie and contribute to the perception of leadership domain. For example, if business leaders who are easily identified (by their facial features) have very competent-looking faces compared to their less identifiable peers, and also compared to other types of leaders (e.g., sports leaders), this would suggest that people associate facial competence with business leadership (and vice-versa). By contrast, to the extent that a particular evaluation dimension objectively and reliably distinguishes types of leaders, regardless of how stereotypic they look, this variable might be an accurate predictor of leadership domain.

Study 1

Evaluating the accuracy and meta-accuracy of face-based leadership domain inferences

We recruited a large (non-student) sample of (mainly) British participants and presented them with photos of leaders in the U.S. In particular, they were shown pairs of faces drawn from two (of the four) leadership categories (e.g., business leaders and military leaders). On each trial, they were asked to judge which face belongs to a target leadership category (e.g., to identify the business leader in each pair). They also reported their confidence in each judgment. Finally, after each block of trials, they estimated how well they performed. Every participant completed two blocks of trials: one with faces drawn from two of the four leadership categories (e.g., business leaders and military leaders) and a second block with faces drawn from the remaining two domains (e.g., political leaders and sports leaders). We counterbalanced leadership pairings, target categories, and block orderings across participants.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited from Maximiles–U.K. (www.maximiles.co.uk), a British Internet service in which members earn points by completing surveys, which they can then use to purchase various consumer products (see Reimers, 2009, for additional details). Our initial sample consisted of 778 participants (97% from the U.K.). Prior to analyses, we discarded the responses of participants who either failed to complete the study, spent less than 10 min completing the entire study (fewer than 4% did

Analyses

Before carrying out our analyses we discarded trials in which either leader was reportedly recognized (1% of trials, on average). Furthermore, we only considered trials in which the two faces were matched in terms of their facial hair (mustache and/or beard) and whether they wore glasses. We then calculated each participant's judgment accuracy (the percentage of correct judgments), their average confidence level (across trials), and the point-biserial correlation (across trials) between their

Accuracy levels: Estimated vs. actual

Fig. 2 shows how well participants thought they performed and how accurate they actually were at distinguishing leaders in each category pairing (along with the 95% confidence intervals for each mean). Despite being explicitly informed that even random guessing would generally yield chance-level accuracy, participants were rather pessimistic about their performance: mean accuracy estimates were all significantly below chance. And yet, across all leadership judgment tasks, participants performed

Discussion

Our first study shows that human judges can accurately infer several leadership domains from facial cues alone. However, that study tells us very little about the underlying impressions or facial cues that drive their judgments. It is worth reminding our readers that we did control for a number of possibly relevant cues, such as gender, ethnicity, clothing, hairstyle, facial hair, and glasses. Consequently, none of these variables can account for our results. Moreover, the leaders in our study

Participants

We recruited a completely new sample of participants from the (mainly) British online population used in our previous study (Maximiles–U.K.; the link to the current study was only sent to members who had not participated in our first study). Our initial sample consisted of 1105 participants (97% from the U.K.). Prior to analyses, we discarded the responses of participants who either failed to complete the study, spent less than 7 min or more than 60 min completing the entire study (fewer than 4%

Analyses

We standardized ratings within participants (across leader photos), using a z-score transformation based on each participant's mean and standard deviation. We then averaged these z-scores across participants for each leader photo and for each dimension of evaluation. We thus obtained 15 average z-scores for each leader, corresponding to the 15 dimensions of interest. Table 2 presents the correlations between these 15 dimensions (each one based on n = 80 leader photos). As this table makes

Correlates of leadership domain facial stereotyping

To determine which dimensions of evaluation might drive people's inferences about leadership domain, we can compare how the most stereotypical looking and least stereotypical looking leaders are perceived. Fig. 4 shows the average attractiveness and facial component scores of the most accurately identified and least accurately identified leaders in each of the four domains. The most stereotypic-looking business leaders scored significantly higher on the facial “Competence” dimension than their

Discussion

Our second study identified several basic dimensions of evaluation that correlate with face-based judgments of leadership domain, as well as those that actually predict leadership domain. By combining these two sets of results, we can start to speculate about the facial-evaluations that the participants in our first study might have been using to achieve their above-chance performance (when trying to infer leadership domain from faces). The results of our second study suggest that people may

General discussion

Our results show that leaders from several different domains are distinguishable by their facial features. Specifically, we found that British participants could identify American business leaders (company CEOs), military leaders (U.S. Army Generals), and sports leaders (American football coaches), from a ‘lineup’ of two faces (belonging to leaders from two different domains) with above-chance accuracy. They were able to do so despite not recognizing either face and even though these leaders

Conclusion

People seem to be surprisingly good at inferring leadership domain from facial cues, yet surprisingly bad at evaluating their ability to do so. These findings have clear implications for leadership perception and selection, yet more research is needed to better understand the potential impact that this kind of facial stereotyping might have on leaders and their followers.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Newton International Fellowship from the Royal Society and The British Academy (to C.Y.O.). In addition, we gratefully acknowledge the financial assistance of the Behavioural Science Global Research Priorities program at the University of Warwick. We thank Vanshika Agarwala, Tyson Hayes, Alexander Mushore, Maria Okun, and Aditi Somani for providing research assistance. Finally, we would like to thank Panu Poutvaara and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful

References (63)

  • S.T. Hunter et al.

    First and ten leadership: A historiometric investigation of the CIP leadership model

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (2011)
  • G.S. Ligon et al.

    Qualifying leader lives: What historiometric approaches can tell us

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (2012)
  • G.S. Ligon et al.

    Development of outstanding leadership: A life narrative approach

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (2008)
  • R.G. Lord et al.

    A test of leadership categorization theory: Internal structure, information processing, and leadership perceptions

    Organizational Behavior and Human Performance

    (1984)
  • M.D. Mumford et al.

    The sources of leader violence: A multi-level comparison of ideological and non-ideological leaders

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (2007)
  • L.R. Offermann et al.

    Implicit leadership theories: Content, structure, and generalizability

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (1994)
  • C.Y. Olivola et al.

    Fooled by first impressions? Reexamining the diagnostic value of appearance-based inferences

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2010)
  • G.A. Pfann et al.

    Business success and businesses' beauty capital

    Economics Letters

    (2000)
  • P. Poutvaara et al.

    Faces of politicians: Babyfacedness predicts inferred competence but not electoral success

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2009)
  • B.R. Spisak et al.

    Facing the situation: Testing a biosocial contingency model of leadership in intergroup relations using masculine and feminine faces

    The Leadership Quarterly

    (2012)
  • J. Antonakis

    Predictors of leadership: The usual suspects and the suspect traits

  • D. Ames et al.

    Not so fast: The (not-quite-complete) dissociation between accuracy and confidence in thin slice impressions

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2010)
  • J. Antonakis et al.

    Predicting elections: Child's play!

    Science

    (2009)
  • J. Antonakis et al.

    The far side of leadership: Rather difficult to face

  • B.M. Bass et al.

    The Bass handbook of leadership: Theory, research, and managerial applications

    (2008)
  • C.C. Ballew et al.

    Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA

    (2007)
  • D.J. Brown

    In the minds of followers: Follower-centric approaches to leadership

  • B.J. Calder

    An attribution theory of leadership

  • D.F. Caldwell et al.

    Measuring person–job fit with a profile-comparison process

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1990)
  • L.J. Chapman et al.

    Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1969)
  • P.D. Cherulnik et al.

    Physical appearance and leadership: Exploring the role of appearance-based attribution in leader emergence

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • Cited by (73)

    • Audit partner facial traits, gender, and career outcomes

      2024, Accounting, Organizations and Society
    • Chief executive officers' appearance predicts company performance, or does it? A replication study and extension focusing on CEO successions

      2023, Leadership Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      Second, another line of research aimed to identify objective facial features that drive perceptions of leadership ability and, hence, may account for the link between appearance and performance. Livingston and Pearce (2009) examined the role of “babyfaceness” in the performance of White and Black CEOs and found that Black CEOs with more babyish facial features tended to earn higher salaries and lead more prestigious companies than more mature-faced Black CEOs (for further findings on the role of babyfaceness in the perception of business leaders, see Olivola, Eubanks, and Lovelace (2014)). In turn, Wong, Ormiston, and Haselhuhn (2011) postulated that a CEO's success (as measured by financial performance) may be determined by facial width-to-height-ratio (WHR).

    • Gender stereotypes explain different mental prototypes of male and female leaders

      2022, Leadership Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition to people’s global leader prototypes, people may also hold distinct cognitive representations of leaders across the domains they lead. Examining perceptions of male leaders from politics (i.e., US state Governors), business (i.e., company CEOs), sports (i.e., American football coaches), and the military (i.e., U.S. Army Generals), one study paired two male leaders' faces from different domains and asked participants to select which belonged to a specific type of leader domain (Olivola, Eubanks, et al., 2014; Olivola, Funk, et al., 2014). They found people could accurately distinguish sports leaders from business leaders (but not politicians) based on different trait evaluations.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Disclaimer: Any opinion expressed in whole or in part are those of the authors and do not represent those, nor reflect the official policy, of the Office of Naval Research, Department of the Army, or the US Department of Defense.

    1

    Tel.: + 44 24765 24985.

    2

    Tel.: + 1 845 938 5002.

    View full text